Applied Computational Intelligence and So Computing
T:Isolateprimitivesbymslevel.
ms ms
ms
P
A C C E
A2 A B B C
C D
A A2 A B
P
A B C D A1 A3 B C D
A1 A3
P
(none) B C C D
A B C
P
A B5 C1 C5
D
B B B3 B5 C1
C C5 D D
A B3 B C5
Note. Primitives in bold face were persistent at two or three ms levels.
P involved nonadjacent rows (A-D/ and B-D/ on P; B-D/
on P; and A-C/, A-D/, and B-D/ on P, where denotes
the number of cases), whereas only AC out of six patterns
did so on P. e strictly vertical patterns were limited to
columns and (B-D/ on P; B-C/ and B-D/ on P; A-
B/, A-D/, and B-D/ on P; and B-C/ and C-D/ on P). e
rest were rightward on P and P, leward on P, or mixed
on P.
Among all of the patterns in Table , the heterogeneous
sequences were mostly unilateral in that the symmetric pairs
were limited in number (BB-BB on P; BB-BB on P;
AA-AA, AB-BA, AC-CA, and BB-BB on
P; and none on P). Four of these were horizontal sequences.
e constituents were limited to a subset consisting of the rst
three rows and columns, that is, {A2, B1,B2,B3, and C3}.
e individual constituents of the multichunk patterns
were frequent by themselves as primitive patterns at a given
ms level, but not vice versa. Table lists the isolate primitive
patterns not participating in any multichunk pattern at a
given ms level. While the number of total primitive patterns
monotonically decreased from ms to ms, the ratio of
the isolate primitive patterns to the total primitive patterns
monotonically increased on all pages almost perfectly. e
ratios at ms12,14,16 were 4/17,7/11,4/5, 4/13,5/8,2/4,
0/13,4/11, 3/6,and5/17,9/14,4/6, in the order of P, P,
P, and P. e sole exception was the second and the third
ratiosonP.erewerenoisolatesonPatms.
Generally, an isolate primitive at a given ms level would
become a member of sequence(s) at a lower level and would
notbepresentatahigherlevel.Exceptionally,C,located
in the rightmost column, persisted on P as an isolate at all
ms levels. Partial persistence was observed between ms and
ms on P (A), P (A, A), and P (B) as well as between
ms and ms on P (B, C). No persistence was observed
on P. e persistent ones on P and P were limited to the
rst three columns of the top row, {A1,A2,A3},whereasthose
on P spread over rows B and C in columns , , and , that is,
{B3,B5, C1,C5}.
Finally, E on P at ms was the sole frequent segment in
the bottom row E where segments were generally infrequent
across pages at all ms levels.
4. Discussion
Eye-tracking researchers have inferred a xation from gaze
points closely clustered in space and time, treating it as a
meaningful unit of information processing, that is, a chunk,
a familiar concept in psychology. Chunking of lower-level
chunks into a higher one is not uncommon as seen in the rela-
tionships letter,word,phrase,sentence,paragraph,....e
present paper examined the patterns of second-order chunks,
that is, chunks of xations, using isolate gaze point(s) not
participating in any xation as the delimiter. e delimiter
wasassumedtoplayanauxiliaryroleinchunking,likea
pause in speech.
Most of the identied chunks were short, consisting of
one or two xations. Also, the transitions within multixation
chunks and between chunks were mostly short in distance,
either loops or one-block shis to adjacent segments. ese
seem to be attributable to the minimum criterion of the deli-
miter we employed—at least one isolate gaze point. Hence,
even an accidental dislocation of one’s gaze resulted in chunk-
ing. It would be ideal if we could separate cognitively mean-
ingful chunking from accidental chunking. Until an eective
method is established, the best we can do is to be cautious in
interpreting the results.
Actually, setting an appropriate criterion is a dicult
task due to the possible individual and situational variations.
Perhaps individuated criteria will be appropriate instead of a
uniform criterion. Further investigation of the distributions
of gaze points participating in xations and those that are
isolated is necessary.
As reported earlier, within- and between-chunk transi-
tions were similar in that the rst two modal distances were
zero (i.e., loops) and one block. However, these diered in
order and in magnitude. Loops were primary among within-
chunk transitions but secondary among between-chunk tran-
sitions. e opposite was true for the one-block shis. Next,
theproportionsoftheprimaryandsecondarydistancesofthe
within-chunk transitions exceeded the respective propor-
tions pertaining to the between-chunk transitions. Similarly,
there were more long-distance shis between chunks than
within them.
eseresultsseemtosuggestthattheattentionofoursub-
jects was most likely shied, aer a pause, to an adjacent seg-
mentoneblockawayorwithinthesamesegment.emed-
ium or long-distance shis were also separated by pauses,
though their proportions were smaller than the short ones.
Shis without a pause, that is, within-chunk shis, were short,
chiey occurring in the same segment or between adjacent
segments one block away.
Now we turn to a discussion of the frequent patterns (i.e.,
subsequences) extracted by PrexSpan. e patterns were
simple in structure, mostly consisting of single or double
chunks. Furthermore, the chunks themselves contained sin-
gle xations or single loops as expected from the chunk pro-
perties discussed above. More complex structures might have
resulted if we had employed less stringent criteria for the
delimiter. Even so, beneath the structural simplicity, interest-
ing properties emerged as to the segment dierentiation and
the directional unevenness in attentional shis.
First, the within-chunk loops were limited to (A..), (B..),
and (D..), all of which were in the lemost column. While
thepresenceof(D..)wasquitelimited,theleadingrolesof
(A..) and (B..) as prexes in the multichunk sequences are